A different point of hue.
نویسندگان
چکیده
A distinguished line-up of scholars recently got together to stir up discussion about the physiological basis for color and have, with a simple manipulation of decades-old data, challenged one of the fundamental tenets of our current understanding of the neurobiology of color (1). Understanding color is not easy. Newton made some headway, but his demonstration of color’s physical basis provided only limited insight because, as Young (2) pointed out, there simply is not enough space for a receptor for each of the seven million or so perceivable colors at each retinal location. Young argued for a triplet color code, and we now know that such a code exists in the form of the three cone types (Fig. 1). Young’s idea made color a construction of the brain, not a physical attribute, and paved the way for opponent color theory (3) in which color is determined not by trichromacy but by three opponent processes: red-green, blueyellow, and black-white. This theory gained ground because it accounted for the fact that we are unable to see a continuous mixture of ‘‘reddish-greens’’ and ‘‘bluish-yellows,’’ which should be perceivable if color were simply trichromatic. In the 1960s, De Valois et al. (4) discovered that many cells in the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) (the thalamic relay from the retina to primary visual cortex) show chromatic opponency. LGN cells inherit this property from retinal ganglion cells. Some are excited by red and inhibited by green (R G ); others are excited by blue and inhibited by yellow (B Y ); others are excited by white and inhibited by black (W Blk ). R G , Y B , and Blk W cells also exist. It was natural to suggest that these cells are the basis for opponent colors, and most neuroscientists today accept some version of this view: the three cone types embody trichromatic theory, and the chromatically opponent LGN cells, each receiving specific cone inputs, represent opponent theory, although the purity of the cone inputs is disputed (5–7). In a recent issue of PNAS, Romney et al. (1) have done two straightforward things to shake up this interpretation. First, they plotted the spectral reflectance from the 1269 Munsell color chips§ in ‘‘cone space.’’ Cone space is often used to show the relative input of the three cone types to neurons, but Romney et al. used it to show how each Munsell color chip would activate the three cone types under standard illumination (Fig. 2A). Cells in the parvocellular layers of the LGN are clustered in cone space: they receive on average balanced opponent input from L versus M cones (8). As Romney et al. point out, the Munsell chips are not randomly distributed when plotted in cone space, but they cluster like LGN cells. The second thing Romney et al. (1) did was to plot in color space the spectral sensitivity of the 147 LGN cells measured by De Valois et al. (4) (Fig. 2B). Color space uses perceptual coordinates of hue and saturation. Romney et al. treated the spectral sensitivities of the LGN cells as reflectance spectra, enabling a direct comparison with Munsell reflectance spectra. According to Munsell’s original aim, the color chips populate the entire color space. Curiously, the LGN data seem to do so, too, despite being clustered in cone space (Fig. 2). The clustering of LGN cells in cone space has been taken as proof that LGN cells represent the physiological implementation of opponent color theory. Romney et al. (1) ask how we can keep this conclusion if (i) the Munsell color chips also cluster in cone space and (ii) the LGN cells do not cluster when their spectral sensitivities are plotted in perceptual space. Well, they say, we cannot: ‘‘The LGN cells are more or less evenly distributed in perceptual space by ganglion cells that aggregate cone receptor responses in a large variety of combinations that represent all areas of the [color] space.’’ This punchy conclusion is going to stir some hearts because it threatens to undermine the idea that parvocellular cells embody opponent-color axes and, more generally, aims once again to make color a physical attribute tied to reflectance spectra. But should we be surprised that the Munsell color chip data cluster in the same pattern as the LGN cells in plots of cone space? Well, maybe not. Munsell’s color scheme has successfully standardized color and is used in almost all jobs that require color identification. Munsell used his own visual system in developing his system, so it might not be a coincidence that the reflectance spectra of Munsell’s color chips match the sensitivity of the LGN cells if LGN cells serve as the building blocks for color discrimination. But Romney et al. (1) assert that reflectance spectra of natural objects, not just the Munsell spectra, also cluster in cone space just like the Munsell data (A. K. Romney, personal communication). Does this mean that the nonrandom distribution of LGN responses in cone space is an adaptation to the same nonrandom distribution of reflectance spectra in the world and not the brain’s implementation of opponent color theory? We don’t think so. For starters, the clustering of Munsell chips in cone space is not nearly as tight as that of LGN cells.
منابع مشابه
A Study of Relationship Between Color Inconstancy Level and CIELAB Color Coordinates
In commercial point of view, it is of great interest to find colors with high color constancy level. Color constancy as a relatively property of object’s appearance could be affected by different factors. In this article, the color inconstancy index of CMCCON02 and CIELAB color coordinate of 1269 samples of Munsell at CIELchº color system to study any correlations, was calculated. The correspon...
متن کاملRecognition of Sequence of Print and Ink Strokes: Investigation the Effect of Handwriting Pressure, Hue of Ink, Printer and Paper Type
By introducing of digital techniques, forensic document examiners has been encouraged to work with better accuracy in non-destructive ways. The aim of this study was to present a non-destructive, accessible, economic (affordable), user friendly, portable, useful and easy technique for specifying the order of crossing lines of ink stroke and printed text. The intersections of LaserJet and In...
متن کاملA definition of global hue contrast in artworks
In the composition, analysis and evaluation of artworks, an understanding of colour is of high importance. Colour is commonly understood in terms of its perceptual components: hue, saturation and lightness. Of these, it is within the hue values of an artwork that many of their high-level aesthetic dimensions are found and it is through the global contrast of hue by which these dimensions are be...
متن کاملUsing the Geometric Model to Explain the Longitudinal and Cross–Sectional Reflection Behaviors of Acrylic yarns
In the present work the reflection behavior and the color appearance of acrylic yarns, as pile yarns used in carpet and pilled fabrics, are considered along their lengths as well as their cross- sections. Differences between longitudinal and cross-sectional reflection behaviors of yarns are measured in different yarn densities and hues and explained by the geometric model. The results of experi...
متن کاملUsing the Geometric Model to Explain the Longitudinal and Cross–Sectional Reflection Behaviors of Acrylic yarns
In the present work the reflection behavior and the color appearance of acrylic yarns, as pile yarns used in carpet and pilled fabrics, are considered along their lengths as well as their cross- sections. Differences between longitudinal and cross-sectional reflection behaviors of yarns are measured in different yarn densities and hues and explained by the geometric model. The results of experi...
متن کاملSuperior discrimination for hue than for saturation and an explanation in terms of correlated neural noise.
The precision of human colour discrimination depends on the region of colour space in which measurements are made and on the direction in which the compared colours-the discriminanda-differ. Working in a MacLeod-Boynton chromaticity diagram scaled so that thresholds at the white point were equal for the two axes, we made measurements at reference points lying on lines that passed at 45° or -45°...
متن کاملذخیره در منابع من
با ذخیره ی این منبع در منابع من، دسترسی به آن را برای استفاده های بعدی آسان تر کنید
برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید
ثبت ناماگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید
ورودعنوان ژورنال:
- Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
دوره 102 31 شماره
صفحات -
تاریخ انتشار 2005